![]() |
If this is your first visit, be sure to check out the FAQ by clicking the link above. You may have to register before you can post: click the register link above to proceed. To start viewing messages, select the forum that you want to visit from the selection below. |
|
|
Thread Tools | Display Modes |
|
#1
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
It's pretty well established that soaring birds have extremely good
eyesight. I think they just see the ground moving away when they are climbing. Thermals also have a lot of stuff in them like insects and seeds. The birds are probably able to see these rising at a considerable distance There's a lot of visual cues while thermalling. If you are trying to thermal near a mountain peak it's easy to see the glider rise at the onset of lift. The varios will signal lift two or three seconds later. BTW, that story about the USA hang glider team chasing buzzards and landing next to a dead cow is really funny. Bill Daniels "Derrick Steed" wrote in message ... It's a well known fact that birds bones are very light and filled with holes, just like we have sinuses in our head bones. I've pondered how birds might sense rate of climb many times and I now hold the view that they sense it via the cavities in their bones - this would provide them with a very sensitive variometer, the capacity being automatically incorporated so to speak. I'm also convinced that birds soar for pleasure as well as because they might have to (e.g. Pelicans soar when they are migrating and follow similar climb/glide patterns to us). I once observed a seagull from the restaurant at the top of the OMPI building in Geneva - a seagull was already soaring near the ITU building when suddenly another shot past the window in a fast glide headed straight for a point below the other seagull, when it got there it pulled up into the climb underneath the other gull turning in the same direction. Obviously his/her CSI (Chief Seagull Instructor) had made the point about proper thermal entry. Rgds, Derrick Steed Does anyone have an idea of how the birds know where to thermal? Do they have a vario? Where is it? Where is its capacity? Assuming they breath while thermalling, then I doubt they use their lungs as capacity... or maybe they stop and sense the air coming out their noses. Just wondering... Uri 4XGJC (Andy Durbin) wrote in message news:... "Bill Daniels" wrote in message news:... Most hawks circling low are looking for rodents, not lift. Bill Daniels But many times I have shared thermals with Hawks at high altitude. How did they get there if not by working thermals at low altitude? I have never been in a thermal with a Red Tailed Hawk that didn't seem to be trying to optimize climb rate. Turkey Vultures are a different story. They seem to be happy with any sloppy thermal technique as long as they maintain altitude. Andy |
#2
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Bill Daniels wrote:
It's pretty well established that soaring birds have extremely good eyesight. I think they just see the ground moving away when they are climbing. Good eyesight is one thing, precision at judging distance another, the latter basically dependent on how far apart your eyes are. No way can they judge their climb rate based on seeing the ground moving away. Thermals also have a lot of stuff in them like insects and seeds. The birds are probably able to see these rising at a considerable distance That could be, and other visual cues as well. CV |
#3
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
C'mon, now. Binocular depth perception ends for humans at about 20 feet and
is only really useful up to arms length, yet we can still judge distance well. Since the bird is moving, they can use dynamic field depth perception that has nothing to do with interocular distance. Close one eye and move your head back and forth or up and down. You will see what I mean. Birds and other small animals are observed to move their heads constantly to better judge distances. Ever ride one of those glass elevators (lifts) on the outside of a tall building? Did you notice how powerful the impression of climbing is? I still claim that they can see themselves rise away from the ground. Excellent vision and the experience to use it to the fullest is the likely explanation. It's the simplest explanation and requires no internal vario. Bill Daniels "CV" wrote in message news ![]() Bill Daniels wrote: It's pretty well established that soaring birds have extremely good eyesight. I think they just see the ground moving away when they are climbing. Good eyesight is one thing, precision at judging distance another, the latter basically dependent on how far apart your eyes are. No way can they judge their climb rate based on seeing the ground moving away. Thermals also have a lot of stuff in them like insects and seeds. The birds are probably able to see these rising at a considerable distance That could be, and other visual cues as well. CV |
#4
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Bill Daniels wrote:
C'mon, now. Binocular depth perception ends for humans at about 20 feet and is only really useful up to arms length, yet we can still judge distance well. Since the bird is moving, they can use dynamic field depth perception that has nothing to do with interocular distance. Close one eye and move your head back and forth or up and down. You will see what I mean. Birds and other small animals are observed to move their heads constantly to better judge distances. Ever ride one of those glass elevators (lifts) on the outside of a tall building? Did you notice how powerful the impression of climbing is? I still claim that they can see themselves rise away from the ground. Excellent vision and the experience to use it to the fullest is the likely explanation. It's the simplest explanation and requires no internal vario. I can't believe that, except for very low heights. The elevator experience you mention mention is for such heights, or at least when something (the building itself) is very near. The best processing system (e.g. the bird's brain) cannot infer anything from missing or non significative input. In the case of climbing, the only information on which you say they rely is the change in the apparent size of ground features. I didn't do the computation, but I bet that the change during one full turn is below the optical resolution of a bird's eye. In this domain, we are better equiped than they are, our eyes are larger. Nevertheless we can't decide if a glider or a bird is climbing when watching them from below just by watching the change of their size during a short time, except when they are very low. However I agree that after watching a bird for a long time, as it changed from a beautiful thing with discernable separate feathers at the tips to a vanishing little point in the sky, I can say that it was climbing. |
#5
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article , Robert Ehrlich
I can't believe that, except for very low heights. The elevator experience you mention mention is for such heights, or at least when something (the building itself) is very near. The best processing system (e.g. the bird's brain) cannot infer anything from missing or non significative input. In the case of climbing, the only information on which you say they rely is the change in the apparent size of ground features. I didn't do the computation, but I bet that the change during one full turn is below the optical resolution of a bird's eye. In this domain, we are better equiped than they are, our eyes are larger. On the other hand, do you think you'd be able to spot a mouse from 3000ft? No problem for some birds of prey. -- Mike Lindsay |
#6
|
|||
|
|||
![]() In article , Robert Ehrlich I can't believe that, except for very low heights. The elevator experience you mention mention is for such heights, or at least when something (the building itself) is very near. The best processing system (e.g. the bird's brain) cannot infer anything from missing or non significative input. In the case of climbing, the only information on which you say they rely is the change in the apparent size of ground features. I didn't do the computation, but I bet that the change during one full turn is below the optical resolution of a bird's eye. In this domain, we are better equiped than they are, our eyes are larger. You don't look down to see height changes, you look out at an angle. You're not looking for changes in the size of objects, you look for changes in angles. It's just like we judge height on final approach to landing. I can judge the strength of thermals visually up to 1000 meters or so and I bet the birds can do a lot better. Bill Daniels |
#7
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Bill Daniels wrote:
You don't look down to see height changes, you look out at an angle. You're not looking for changes in the size of objects, you look for changes in angles. It's just like we judge height on final approach to landing. I can judge the strength of thermals visually up to 1000 meters or so and I bet the birds can do a lot better. Bill Daniels Well, the changes in (apparent) size of objets is nothing else than a change in an angle. I agree that looking for such an angle just below the glider is not what will maximize the change for a given height change. If your method is by watching the change in the angle of the directions of some fixed ground feature and the horizon, it can easily be shown that the maximum rate of change is obtained by looking at some feature at 45 degrees below the horizon. In this case, the change rate, in radians per climbing meter, is 1/(2*height), at 1000 meters the rate of change is of 1.7 minute per meter, in order to see a 1 degree change when climbing at 2 m/s, you have to wait 35 seconds. Difficult but workable. I should try it in my next flight, although I think I will not be able to perceive changes below 10 degrees, when looking at 45 degrees. Looking toward a more horizontal direction should provide better senitivity, as the fixed feature and the horizon are together in the visual field, but the rate of change of the angle is much lower. When looking in the same direction as on final approach, i.e. the direction of the 1/10 slope, the rate of change is 1/(10*height) radians per climbing meter, 5 times lower than at 45 degrees, you have to wait nearly 3 minutes climbing at 2 m/s to see a 1 degree change at 1000 m. |
Thread Tools | |
Display Modes | |
|
|
![]() |
||||
Thread | Thread Starter | Forum | Replies | Last Post |
Changes in Instrument Proficiency Check Requirements | Richard Kaplan | Instrument Flight Rules | 71 | June 10th 04 08:02 PM |
Kerry begins circling the drain: | Tarver Engineering | Military Aviation | 0 | February 12th 04 06:04 PM |
What determines LNAV "circling mode"? | Jack | Soaring | 11 | November 20th 03 05:15 AM |
Circling To Land On NDB-B and NDB-C | S. Ramirez | Instrument Flight Rules | 7 | July 13th 03 03:46 PM |